Epoch TOPICS CONTEXTS PERSPECTIVES ACTIVITIES METHODS FIGURES HELP TIME LINE Acknowledgements ACTIVITY 3 Exploring persepctives ACTIVITY 1 Using the timeline ACTIVITY 2 Using the biographies ACTIVITY 5 Using the figures, methods, perspectives, topics and context icons ACTIVITY 4 Exploring Topics Ainsworth Allport Baddeley Baron-Cohen Asperger Asch Binet Bartlett Bilig Belbin Bowlby Bruce Buss Cattell Ceci Byrne Bruner Bryant Cohen Cosmides Chomsky Cooper Charcot Conway Damasio Darwin Costa Dawkins Csikszentmihalyi Crick Erikson Eysenck Ekman Descartes Ebbinghaus Dennet Frith Freud Anna Freud Sigmund Falschung Fodor Festinger Goffman Gibson Goodall Galton Goldberg Gathercole Gregory Humphrey James Heider Janet Goodman Kahneman Lazarus Jung Kanner Klein Kelly Mayo McCrae Luria Loftus Lorenz Maslow Neisser Norman Morton Milgram Milner Mead Potter Plomin Piaget Pinker Penfield Pavlov Tajfel Sperry Skinner Saywitz Spears Rogers Triesman Turner Tulving Tooby Taylor Thorndike Weiskrantz Vrij Aldert Warrington Watson Vygotsky Tversky Wundt Zimbardo Whiten Wetherell You can check your answers against ours You can check your answers against ours You can check your answers against ours You can check your answers against ours You can check your answers against ours

Map Node Icon: BryantP.JPG Bryant url anchor

Views:  FIGURES, TIME LINE, Baron-Cohen, Gregory, Luria, Piaget

Reference Node Icon: BryantP.JPG  url anchor

Note Node Bryant, Peter url anchor

Answer Node Influenced by url anchor

Map Node Icon: PIAGET.jpg Piaget url anchor

Views:  FIGURES, TIME LINE, Binet, Bryant, Goodman, Saywitz

Reference Node Icon: red-16.png Cognitive development url anchor

Cognitive development refers to the gradual unfolding of the child's abilities to think, to reason, and to use language. At the other end of the life-span, cognitive development looks at how certain cognitive capacities such as memory can diminish with age. url anchor
Views:  TOPICS, Ceci, Bruner, Bryant, Cohen, Plomin, Piaget, Saywitz, Vygotsky

Map Node Icon: Baron-cohen.jpg Baron-Cohen url anchor

Views:  FIGURES, TIME LINE, Bryant, Frith, Loftus, Neisser, Whiten

Map Node Icon: LURIA.jpg Luria url anchor

Views:  FIGURES, TIME LINE, Bryant, Freud Sigmund, Pavlov, Vygotsky

Reference Node Icon: green-16.png Logintudinal url anchor

Longitudinal. Like age-based cross-sectional methods, this approach is designed to provide data on how people change over time. However, it overcomes the methodological problems of cross-sectional studies by making repeated measures on the same group of people over long periods of time. This approach also allows researchers to make detailed observations of major points of change in people's lives (if the times between observations are relatively short). This research would typically take place over a period of some years (or even decades). Although it is a very powerful way of looking at age-related changes, it clearly also involves a great commitment in time on behalf of the researchers (and willingness on the part of their participants to continue participating in the research). Perhaps the best known longitudinal studies are cohort studies that aim to follow people born in one week from birth throughout their lives. url anchor
Views:  METHODS, Bryant

Reference Node Icon: blue-16.png Social-cons url anchor

Social Constructionist. The basic idea underlying all social constructionist theories is that the ways in which we understand the world and the things we consider true are not just 'natural' ways of understanding reality, but are actively constructed between people as they go about their everyday lives and interact with each other. These 'ways of knowing', therefore, are inextricably part of a social process. This perspective suggests that all human knowledge – even knowledge that seems to be just an objective description of 'reality', such as physics or chemistry – is in fact constructed by people within their own particular historical and social contexts. Social constructionism falls within the broad approach to studying social psychology called 'sociological social psychology' (SSP). This fundamentally disagrees with the approach taken by experimental social psychology, which tackles social psychology by establishing the principles governing individual behaviour, and then seeing how these are modified in a social setting, a position called 'psychological social psychology' (PSP). (See EXPERIMENTAL SOCIAL PSYCHOLOGY). SSP approaches have quite a different focus. Instead of seeing the social context as simply another 'variable', they ask questions such as 'in a particular social setting, how do the social and cultural practices actually act to construct the individual, as he or she develops from childhood?'. The social worlds we live in are seen as 'interpretive networks', continually being constructed and reconstructed by individuals, groups, and institutions in interaction with each other. \n\nSocial constructionists are interested in how individual behaviour is inextricably linked with its social context, and in fact frequently emerges from that context. Most of human action is seen as social interaction, modifying even those of our activities which we may think we have created at a purely individual level, or which arise from 'biological instincts'. This viewpoint tends to make social constructionists critical of perspectives such as humanistic, with its much greater emphasis on the individual's capacity to create and modify their own world view. However, it would be a mistake to see social constructionism as taking a position of 'sociological determinism', seeing people as simple pawns of their social context. People are indeed seen by social constructionists as making active choices – however, the range of choices available, and how they are conceptualised, is seen as structured by the social materials available (although these 'social materials' are themselves seen as subject to a continual process of renegotiation and change).\nSocial constructionists argue that what is accepted as knowledge at any level – scientific, cultural or social – and in any area, is a social process. Knowledge is therefore seen as an expression of the particular social and historical context in which it is produced - what is taken to be 'true' or 'real' is always produced and sustained by social processes. This is applied especially to current taken-for-granted knowledge that seems most 'obvious', e.g. the 'natural' distinctions between different genders. Social constructionists would not, of course, deny the existence of clear physiological differences – but what they would be particularly interested in was how these differences are made sense of in different societies, and the implications for people's identity and way of life. They would argue that what most frequently has the greatest impact on people's sense of self and social lives are not what 'exists', but how it is conceptualised in that particular society. Hence a particular emphasis is placed on the role of language in constructing agreed social 'realities'. For example, the experience of being gay within those societies that see it as immoral (and perhaps illegal), compared with those societies that are quite comfortable with gay lifestyles. Note that this doesn't just vary with geography, but also historical period (e.g. compare British laws against male homosexuality nowadays, compared to just forty years ago). Another example might be whether we call someone a 'freedom fighter' or a 'terrorist'. Although these two terms can refer to the same person, each constructs a totally different way of viewing that person and the world. The language we use justifies particular responses to people and to situations, and plays a major role in constructing 'power relations' between people. There is an interesting contrast between social constructionist and psychometric views of identity. Instead of seeing identity as relatively fixed, identities are seen as shifting over time and between different socio-cultural contexts. Identity is therefore seen as historically and culturally specific, and dynamic, in that it is seen as constantly in the process of being 'renegotiated' in social interaction.\nIn contrast to most experimental research, social constructionism tends to use qualitative data, taken from naturalistic settings (i.e. from people's everyday lives and social interactions). This can involve looking at social representations of everyday concepts, or analysis of people's 'discourses' (i.e. those ways of thinking and talking about issues which are currently available in an individual's particular culture). These discourses are seen as the processes by which people construct meanings, and their study is called discourse analysis. url anchor
Views:  PERSPECTIVES, Bilig, Bruner, Bryant, Goffman, Potter, Spears, Vrij Aldert , Vygotsky, Wetherell

Note Node In 1974 I published a book called Perception and Understanding in Young Children in which I argued that children's first steps in cognition are based on their ability to make relative perceptual judgements and to make inferences by co-ordinating separate relative judgements. url anchor

Note Node My work on mathematics is also done with Terezinha Nunes. In a recent book on this topic we have set out a theory about additive and multiplicative reasoning, in which we argue that both kinds of reasoning have their roots in every day activities, but that children have to learn about cultural practices which make these forms of reasoning more powerful and more sophisticated. url anchor

Note Node I did my first degree in Psychology at Cambridge in 1961, and my PhD on learning in children with learning difficulties at the Institute of Psychiatry in 1963. In 1964-65 I did a post-doctoral stint in Geneva with Piaget. url anchor

Note Node Written by: Peter Bryant url anchor

Note Node I continued my work on children with learning difficulties in London until I came to the Department of Experimental Psychology in Oxford in 1967. Since then I have studied perceptual and cognitive development in children, with particular reference to what they have to learn at school. I have also done some work on perceptual and cognitive processes in infants in their first year. url anchor

Note Node In recent years I have concentrated on how children learn to read and to do mathematics. My work wit h Lynette Bradley and Usha Goswami on reading, has demonstrated a powerful causal link between children's phonological awareness and their success in reading. In other research on reading Terezinha Nunes and I have shown the importance of children's morphological knowledge in the later stages of learning to read and spell. url anchor

Note Node Terezinha Nunes and I are currently doing research on the role of implicit and explicit learning in the acquisition of these cultural practices, which are an essential part of education. In this work we are as concerned with the teachers' implicit and explicit knowledge as with the pupils'. url anchor

Top